New research finds that people given advice by top performers thought that it helped them more, even though it usually didn't.
From: Association for Psychological Science
July 15, 2022 -- When you want advice to achieve
something, whom would you rather ask: the top performer in that area or someone
barely scraping by? Most people would choose the top performer. That person's
advice, however, may not be any more helpful.
"Skillful
performance and skillful teaching are not always the same thing, so we
shouldn't expect the best performers to necessarily be the best teachers as
well," said David Levari (Harvard Business School), lead author of a
recent Psychological Science article.
Across four studies, he
and coauthors APS Fellows Daniel T. Gilbert (Harvard University) and Timothy D.
Wilson (University of Virginia) found that top performers don't give better
advice than other performers, at least in some domains. Rather, they just give
more of it.
"People seem to
mistake quantity for quality," the researchers wrote. "Our studies
suggest that in at least in some instances, people may overvalue advice from
top performers."
In the first study,
Levari and colleagues set out to determine whether people believe an advisor's
performance is a robust indicator of the quality of their advice.
More than 1,100
participants, sourced through Amazon Mechanical Turk, were told they would be
playing a game called Word Scramble and then answering questions about it.
Shown a board of letters, participants were given 60 seconds to form as many
words as possible. Participants played three rounds, each time with a different
board of letters.
The researchers then
asked participants to choose which advisors they would prefer to receive advice
from to get better at the task. Participants showed a strong preference for the
best performers, regardless of how the question was asked (i.e., in a
free-choice or forced-choice format).
In the second study,
the researchers explored whether the best performers did indeed give the best
advice. They asked 100 "advisors" to play six rounds of Word Scramble,
write advice for future players, and rate the quality of their own advice. The
best performers believed they had given the best advice.
In the same study,
another 2,085 participants were randomly assigned to either an advice or a
no-advice condition. After playing one round of Word Scramble, participants in
the advice condition received direction from a random advisor, then played five
more rounds. The no-advice participants played six rounds without feedback.
Advisees performed better after receiving advice, and they tended to perform
better with each subsequent round. But the advice from the best performers was
no more helpful, on average, than the advice from the other performers. The
researchers conducted a similar study with darts, showing a similar pattern of
results.
"In our
experiments, people given advice by top performers thought that it helped them
more, even though it usually didn't. Surprisingly, they thought this even
though they didn't know anything about the people who wrote their advice,"
said Levari.
The researchers
conducted two more studies to understand why the advice from better performers
seemed better. Two undergraduate research assistants who were blind to the
study's purposes and hypotheses coded the advice for seven properties:
authoritativeness, actionability, articulateness, obviousness, number of
suggestions, "should" suggestions, and "should not"
suggestions. Each property was analyzed for its perceived helpfulness and
perceived improvement.
Only one property --
number of suggestions -- consistently predicted both the perceived helpfulness
and the perceived improvement of the advice. However, there was no correlation
between the number of suggestions and the efficacy of the advice.
"Top performers
didn't write more helpful advice, but they did write more of it, and people in
our experiments mistook quantity for quality," Levari told APS.
So, why wasn't the
advice more helpful? Levari and colleagues have a few ideas.
First, skilled
performers may overlook fundamental advice because "natural talent and
extensive practice have made conscious thought unnecessary. … A natural-born
slugger who has played baseball every day since childhood may not think to tell
a rookie about something they find utterly intuitive, such as balance and
grip," they wrote.
Second, top performers
may not be skilled communicators. "Even when an excellent performer does
have explicit information to share, they may not be especially adept at sharing
it," the researchers wrote. Finally, a large quantity of advice may be
more than what can realistically be implemented.
"We spend a lot of
time and money looking for good advice, whether from coworkers and coaches,
teachers and tutors, or friends and family," said Levari. "The next
time you get advice, you may want to think less about how much of it there was,
and more about how much of it you could actually use."
https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2022/07/220715151026.htm
No comments:
Post a Comment