In psychiatry, Confabulation
(verb: confabulate) is a memory disturbance, defined as the production
of fabricated, distorted or misinterpreted memories about oneself or the world,
without the conscious intention to deceive. Individuals who confabulate present
incorrect memories ranging from "subtle alterations to bizarre
fabrications", and are generally very confident about their recollections,
despite contradictory evidence.
Confabulation is distinguished from lying as there is no intent to deceive and the person is unaware the information is false. Although individuals can present blatantly false information, confabulation can also seem to be coherent, internally consistent, and relatively normal.
Most known cases of confabulation are symptomatic of brain damage or dementias, such as aneurysm, Alzheimer's disease, or Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome (a common manifestation of thiamine deficiency caused by alcoholism). Additionally confabulation often occurs in people who are suffering from anticholinergic toxidrome when interrogated about bizarre or irrational behavior.
Confabulated memories of all types most often occur in autobiographical memory, and are indicative of a complicated and intricate process that can be led astray at any point during encoding, storage, or recall of a memory. This type of confabulation is commonly seen in Korsakoff's syndrome.
Distinctions
Two types of confabulation are often distinguished:
Another distinction is that between:
Confabulation is associated with several characteristics:
Description
Confabulation is distinguished from lying as there is no intent to deceive and the person is unaware the information is false. Although individuals can present blatantly false information, confabulation can also seem to be coherent, internally consistent, and relatively normal.
Most known cases of confabulation are symptomatic of brain damage or dementias, such as aneurysm, Alzheimer's disease, or Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome (a common manifestation of thiamine deficiency caused by alcoholism). Additionally confabulation often occurs in people who are suffering from anticholinergic toxidrome when interrogated about bizarre or irrational behavior.
Confabulated memories of all types most often occur in autobiographical memory, and are indicative of a complicated and intricate process that can be led astray at any point during encoding, storage, or recall of a memory. This type of confabulation is commonly seen in Korsakoff's syndrome.
Distinctions
Two types of confabulation are often distinguished:
- Provoked (momentary, or secondary) confabulations
represent a normal response to a faulty memory, are common in both amnesia
and dementia, and can become apparent during memory tests.
- Spontaneous (or primary) confabulations do not occur in
response to a cue and seem to be involuntary. They are relatively rare,
more common in cases of dementia, and may result from the interaction
between frontal lobe pathology and organic amnesia.
Another distinction is that between:
- Verbal confabulations, spoken false memories and
are more common, and
- Behavioral confabulations, occur when an individual
acts on their false memories.
Signs and Symptoms
Confabulation is associated with several characteristics:
- Typically verbal statements but can also be
non-verbal gestures or actions.
- Can include autobiographical and non-personal
information, such as historical facts, fairytales, or other aspects of
semantic memory.
- The account can be fantastic or coherent.
- Both the premise and the details of the
account can be false.
- The account is usually drawn from the
patient’s memory of actual experiences, including past and current
thoughts.
- The patient is unaware of the accounts’
distortions or inappropriateness, and is not concerned when errors are
pointed out.
- There is no hidden motivation behind the
account.
- The patient’s personality structure may play
a role in their readiness to confabulate.
Confidence in False Memories
Confabulation of
events or situations may lead to an eventual acceptance of the confabulated
information as true. For instance, people who knowingly lie about a situation
may eventually come to believe that their lies are truthful with time. In an
interview setting, people are more likely to confabulate in situations in which
they are presented false information by another person, as opposed to when they
self-generate these falsehoods. Further, people are more likely to accept false
information as true when they are interviewed at a later time (after the event
in question) than those who are interviewed immediately or soon after the
event. Affirmative feedback for confabulated responses is also shown to
increase the confabulator’s confidence in their response. For instance, in
culprit identification, if a witness falsely identifies a member of a line-up,
he will be more confident in his identification if the interviewer provides
affirmative feedback. This effect of confirmatory feedback appears to last over
time, as witnesses will even remember the confabulated information months
later.
No comments:
Post a Comment